My research revolved around a task called the go/no-go association task (GNAT). It is one of many implicit social cognition tasks which in layman terms, measure unconscious thoughts or preferences towards almost anything (objects, people, groups, categories). The theory behind it is from an early learning experience paradigm - the typical past behaviour/events influence future thoughts, behaviours, attitudes, stereotypes. The trick is that you don't even realize that the past is so important.
It's a speed-accuracy thing where you gotta be quick, and how much you score determines your tendency to associate one thing with another (e.g. the though that fruit is good, bugs are bad, Asians are stingy or Australians are laidback). Why is this important? Well, given that you can easily change the numbers/ranking on questionnaires (i.e., cheat) because there's a fear of being viewed as unfavourable (e.g. ranking self as more 'honest' than you really are), these implicit tasks supposedly eliminates those biases/social pressures.
No time to think. Just respond!
Sounds neat right? Like a fool-proof plan. Now we can REALLY know about people's thoughts. However, recently there has been debate on the validity of these tasks. Are they really better than explicit measures (questionnaires etc)? Why then do some implicit results similar to explicit ones? Shouldn't the implicit tell us something more? Jesse Erwin (2007) addresses the validity of the Implicit Association Task (IAT, the most famous of the family of implicit tasks) in an article as posted below:
----------------------------------------------------
The IAT: How and When It Works
By Jesse Erwin
“Left…right…left…right” could be heard echoing from the Hilton Washington’s Military Room during the APS 19th Annual Convention. And although the chorus may have sounded like boot camp exercises to curious passers-by, it was merely APS Fellow Anthony Greenwald, professor of psychology at the University of Washington and recipient of the Society of Experimental Social Psychology’s 2006 Distinguished Scientist Award, administering the Implicit Association Test (IAT) en masse during his Psi Chi Distinguished Speakers address. Throughout his talk, the University of Washington Professor educated his audience about the history and validity of the IAT and, of course, provided the opportunity to experience the IAT firsthand.
“So what is the IAT?” Greenwald asks his audience. “It’s a measure of associative knowledge. And I don’t describe it as a measure of prejudice or bias, although it can be used to measure implicit prejudice or bias.”
The IAT has risen to prominence since Greenwald first published his findings in 1998; the Brief IAT of race has been taken over a million times. On the surface, the IAT asks participants to categorize words or images on a computer screen with a touch of the keyboard. These categorizations begin to require some cognitive gymnastics, however, as categories become combined. The time it takes to sort out stimuli from the combined categories provides some insight into participants’ mental associations.
“How does it work? Well, fairly simply. If two concepts are associated, it is easy to give the same response to exemplars of both,” says Greenwald. That’s a deceptively uncomplicated explanation behind one of contemporary psychology’s most influential research paradigms. “There’s little more theory underlying the IAT than the idea of association between concepts.”
But the provocative implications of the IAT have sparked controversy in both research circles and the mainstream media. So in his address, “Assessing the Validity of Implicit Association Test Measures,” Greenwald came to the IAT’s defense and discussed its psychometric worthiness.
When discussing internal validity, for example, Greenwald says “empirical research demonstrated that there are several things that might get in the way that in fact did not.” Things like participants’ familiarity with the items or lack thereof, which side of the screen categories are presented on, or whether the person is right or left handed have all been mentioned as possible confounds, but haven’t been borne out in research.
Greenwald went on to illustrate the convergent validity of the IAT with self-report using an example from the 2004 presidential election. Implicit attitudes toward each candidate correlated .73 with self-report measures. “That’s quite high,” Greenwald says. “And that’s evidence of convergence.”
Conversely, research on IAT measures of age attitudes have demonstrated evidence of discriminant validity with self report. Greenwald offers this explanation for the dichotomous results: “I think you get convergent validity when both implicit and explicit attitudes are shaped by the same influences, which means they are formed relatively late in life, such as political preferences.” For those attitudes that are formed earlier in life — in particular racial/ethnic, young/old, and male/female stereotypes — IAT results are likely to diverge from the explicit self report measures.
In a time where social desirability confounds are of pervasive concern in psychological research, one of the IAT’s greatest merits appears to be resistance to faking. Studies have demonstrated that participants rarely devise a successful faking strategy. It appears that taking one’s time is the easiest way to doctor results. “It does work,” Greenwald says of the strategy, “but it also tends to be detectable statistically.”
But as with any test, the IAT has its psychometric vulnerabilities. Greenwald describes the elasticity of the IAT, where experiences with exemplars of test categories shortly before the test can alter results. So, according to Greenwald, having a friendly interaction with a black experimenter just before the test will likely dampen evidence of bias.
Greenwald is frank in his assertion that the test-retest reliability of the IAT leaves something to be desired. “The test-retest reliability is okay for research, but not very good for an individual difference measure that you want to be diagnostic of a single person.” In order to boost its reliability, Greenwald and his colleagues resort to the “standard trick” of administering the test several times. At this point the reliability is “getting good, but still not good enough.”
The Brief IAT, and its less time-consuming administration, offers some promise in this arena. “I think we have to look at the test-retest reliability of the Brief IAT in more studies than we have so far. But the average of the first set of a relatively small set of studies that was done was around .50.”
“The last topic is the most interesting one,” Greenwald asserts. “Does the IAT predict anything interesting?” Pointing to a meta-analysis being conducted by Yale University graduate students Andy Poehlman and Eric Uhlmann, Greenwald says that the IAT performs better than self report at predicting behavior. “This IAT has incremental predictive validity relative to self-report. [The results are] statistically significant, and very clearly so in the meta-analysis.”
It’s a good start for a test that has garnered so much attention and will likely be used increasingly in the near future. But there are few certainties when using psychological tools such as the IAT and more research is needed on the topic. The jarring possibilities of how unconscious attitudes, as measured by the IAT, could affect behavior will ensure that the test garners the examination it deserves.
-----------------------------------------------------
Erwin, J. (2007). IAT: How and when it works. Observer, 20, 12.
Disclaimer and Terms of Use
All material on this blog is the work of the author if not specified otherwise. As all tertiary students know (others please be aware), plagarism is a big no-no though admittedly, it's not so much of a big deal in Malaysia. However, this author begs to differ.
Please be reminded that publishing of original material without the author's permission in any medium (e.g. essays, reports, papers, thesis', etc) will warrant legal action.
Feel free to make use of the techniques after consulting a professional. Misuse of content is solely the reader's personal choice. Readers should be warned that the author is not responsible for any liabilities, inconveniences or irregulaties
following misuse.
Please be reminded that publishing of original material without the author's permission in any medium (e.g. essays, reports, papers, thesis', etc) will warrant legal action.
Feel free to make use of the techniques after consulting a professional. Misuse of content is solely the reader's personal choice. Readers should be warned that the author is not responsible for any liabilities, inconveniences or irregulaties
following misuse.
Subscribe to:
Post Comments (Atom)
No comments:
Post a Comment